Pure
Research Submission Process
Pure Research Reports:
'The
Choral Revolution'
by Rececca Singh and Nick Carpenter
'Kinesthetic Transference in Performance'
by Erika Batdorf, Kate Digby and Denise
Fujiwara
'The Unsuspecting Audience'
by Moynan King & Sherri Hay
'The Invitation'
by Moynan King & Sherri Hay
'The
Box'
by Camellia Koo
'Sound Manipulation'
by Cathy Nosaty, Laurel MacDonald & Philip Strong
'Hello!
Sound, Voice and Connection'
by Heather Nicol
'Beneath the Poetry: Magic not Meaning'
by Kate Hennig
'Exploring the Land Between Speaking
and Singing'
by Guillaume Bernardi
'On
Comedy'
by Lois Brown & Liz Pickard
'Theatre
in Music'
by Nick Fraser & Justin Haynes
'Theatre
of Illumination'
by Shadowland Theatre
Read Brian's article on Pure Research from the Canadian Theatre
Review
|
Pure Research Report:
Voice,
Music and Theatrical Narrative by Martin Julien
Report on Pure Research - Toronto
2003 - Facilitator: Martin Julien
The Theatre Centre, Toronto / April 28, 29, May 1, 12, 13, 14, 2003
Participant Partners: Alex Fallis;
Imali Perrera; Brian Quirt
Our team of theatrical researchers took it upon themselves
to do some preliminary experimentation regarding the influence of
live vocal musical sound on the uses and meaning of narrative within
a theatrical context. Although our work must be considered unscientific
and necessarily limited in scope, we were able to successfully identify
certain patterns and effects through our efforts which may provide
a strong template for future exploration. What follows is a précis
of our activities. As an addendum, I will provide a list of our
primary resource materials.
We began our collective journey with a discussion
of what might be called the semiotics of music." Music
has always presented unique challenges to its beleaguered semiologists;
certainly music is both patterned and communicative, but its systematic
meaning remains entirely elusive. What exactly, we asked ourselves,
does music communicate? More specifically, what function can it
fulfill with regards to narrative or character meaning in a theatrical
(storytelling) setting? It is easy to concur with Adornos
famous paradoxical comment on Mahler that music is "a narrative
that narrates nothing".
Moving into our work, though,
we came to agreement on some generally observed principles.
Firstly, we averred that musical sounds are suffused with feeling,
and that emotional responses are related to the interpretation of
the experience of music. Secondly, we recognized that human beings
are symbolic animals who seek to give interpretation and meaning
to any semiotic traces; therefore, music may not be a narrative
but an incitement to make a narrative, to comment, to analyze. Thirdly,
we acknowledged that music retains both strong emotional and memory-based
associative elements for both the singer/player and the hearer.
Lastly, we questioned whether our well-developed Western tone system
of a twelve-partitioned octave consisting of only major and minor
scales leads to a paucity of emotive and narrative choices with
regard to expression.
Sharing Musical Stories.
An initial exercise was to sing each other a song of our own choosing,
and for everyone to then construct simple narratives, or strings
of images, based on what they heard. Two of the pieces chosen had
distinct religious or liturgical connections, and this over-coding
seemed impossible to dismiss, even within our generally secular
little group. A third piece was populist German (Weill), and this
element too was inescapable in its referential implications. It
also seemed to bring more focus to the singer the subject
or interpreter - and instantly created a theatrical frame
through which to intuit meaning. Needless to say, our narrative
responses were as varied and personal as our own memories, associations
and aesthetics; the confluence of major and minor sounding
passages had the usual result of identifying happy or sad
imagery; and there was certainly some evidence of what we might
call tone-painting, when runs up the scale elicited feelings of
rising and runs down of falling, etc. In the end, we affirmed our
suppositions that music could not be strictly programmatic in its
narrative intent or interpretation. Music appeared to embody a distinct
call to memory (both personal and collective), regardless of its
specific historical associations. We all sought to generate meaning
by subjectively interpreting the songs.
Modes and Moods.
We then proceeded to experiment with medieval modes. Through the
simple singing of scales, we discovered that the various modes did
indeed offer a much wider palate in terms of emotional response.
Whereas all minor scales struck us as similarly sad and dark,
and the major keys offered only slight variations in response (the
plainness of C, the brightness of G), the modal scales demonstrated
much more personality to our ears and voices. (Our favourite was
the dark, seductive and sensuous Locrian mode.)
Strict Spatials.
Here we did some basic exploration with the concept of musical intervals
as they relate to the spatial dialectic between two singers. Dividing
the length of our black box theatre space into twenty-four
equal increments (representing two chromatic scales of twelve),
we proceeded to have our two singers work in interval patterns of
fourths, fifths and octaves, both vocally and spatially. I speculated
that the singers would have more difficulty singing intervals while
physically describing counter-intuitive spatial positions (i.e.
moving to a third position while singing a fourth interval),
but this did not turn out to be particularly true. We did find that
it was much more comfortable for the singer to hold a fourth
against an eighth rather than a third against the eighth,
but overall it was the relationship between the singers as co-performers
that was the pre-eminent factor in spatial movement, rather than
the strict arrangements of measured intervals. There was some evidence
that blindfolded singers were able to recreate the musical interval
spatially with fair accuracy (i.e. singing a fifth while moving
to the fifth position on our divided stage), but this had a lot
to do with utilizing the clues of room tone and remembered positioning.
A few principles we discovered:
ascending intervals tended to move singers
apart; descending, to move them together;
a singer would tend to move in relation to
their partners changing intervals, even if they remained on
the tonic;
narrative was quickly imposed by singer and
listener upon the changing spatial relationships, by the simple
fact of bodies moving toward, or apart from, each other in either
consonant or dissonant reflection of the musical intervals ("oh,
he wants to be near her/oh, shes trying to get away from him",
etc).
Same Music, Different Text.
Here we presented the listener with three very different texts,
spoken dramatically on stage, but accompanied by the very same simple
music in the form of vocal duets (plainchant, strict counterpoint)
performed off-stage in the background. The most interesting observation
here was that in instances where the texts mood and subject
seemed analogous to the musics intent, the listener suspended
their logical and schematic mind in favour of their intuitive one.
When it was easy for the listener to step into the thinking of "oh,
this music supports the text Im hearing Ill just
listen to the story", then that was the choice made. If, however,
the two elements seemed disjunctive, then the analytical mind kicked
in, trying to make sense of the reasons why this text and music
were linked dramatically. There was an immediate search for a cohesive
principle. As well, our rather sombre music tended to raise the
status of the stories which were most prosaic, contemporary and
scatological. The role of the invisible singers as narrative elements
in this theatrical format was not raised observationally.
Behind, To the Side, Up Close.
These two experiments each had two components. In the first, a piece
of fairly dense text was read by an invisible speaker behind the
listener while a singer sitting before the listener hummed a wordless,
whimsical tune. Then the spatial relationship was reversed, with
the speaker on stage and the singer behind. A truly fascinating
experiment we found that whatever was in front of the listener
held most of the narrative weight. The text was extremely difficult
to decipher and attend to when placed behind, and the wordless singer
on display became the story. Conversely, it was easy to hear the
story with the speaker in front of us, and the humming became gentle
background music evocative, but unobtrusive. Also, it was
recognized that the listeners inevitably attempted to correlate
the visible singers presence as a character in a plot which
somehow revolved around the opaque storytelling behind them. On
another level, the text behind the listener sometimes seemed to
play the role of music in a more traditional context. Whatever is
in front is narrative; whatever is behind is support.
In the second, a storyteller was placed in an intimate
spatial relationship with the listener, almost knee to knee, whilst
a singer was placed extreme stage left in a crouched position. The
story told to the listener was childlike and transparent, and the
wordless humming of the singer seemed supportive and unobtrusive.
The presentation was then repeated, but this time the singer placed
strange accents and mildly aggressive dynamics on the same tune;
the text was delivered in a near-identical fashion. Whereas the
listener could clearly hear and enjoy the story the first time,
in the second round it became increasingly difficult to concentrate
with the subtly strident singing emanating from farther away. It
was observed that the singers presence became somewhat malevolent,
and that a dark narrative was easily ascribed to the hummers
intentions. Through subtle shifts of musical dynamics, the intimacy
of the storytelling was noticeably undermined.
And for your Entertainment...
Our research team was lucky enough to invite a half-dozen volunteer
listener/spectators into the theatre during our penultimate
session for a twenty minute presentation. For this event, we strung
together a sort of dramatic pastiche of many of the ideas we had
previously explored though we discarded some devices and
brought in new elements, as well. It is important to note that we
consciously strove to employ only the smallest traces of narrative
meaning and movement in our presentation many of the texts,
music and choreography utilized were virtually arbitrary with regard
to their placement and execution (at least from a narrative point
of view). Unsurprisingly, all of our audience participants worked
to give narrative weight to the smallest of semiotic signs. The
absence of a voice in counterpoint; the absence of a body in a doorway;
the satisfying consonance of two people singing together; the confusion
of repeated text in different context; the mystery of half-heard
melodies joined with prosaic dialogue; the challenge of unifying
different actor and musical elements around the entire theatre
all these disparate actions became quick fodder for the invention
and evocation of story and feeling for our audience. Some observations
about this session:
the opening setting of music, text and audience/performer
dialectic tended to set the frame through which meaning would be
generated by the audience, whether that frame related thematically
to the subsequent material or not;
there were consistent attempts made by viewers to search
for thematic unity in the pieces and their presentation;
its much easier to accept and absorb the confluence
of text, music and presentational style if there appeared a unity
of approach (e.g. German song combined with an aggressive harangue
created a confluence of Third Reich imagery in many peoples
minds, though there was no direct contextual relationship between
these elements);
where the music and text seemed disjunctive, much effort
was spent by the audience in working to establish what that juxtaposition
meant (e.g. a scene about suburban hockey parents accompanied by
an invisible singer warbling through a half-heard Roman Catholic
liturgical piece proved particularly disturbing and ambiguous in
its implications);
there was a real desire by most viewers for the two main
singers, who were often featured in isolation and gazing at the
absence of the other, to come together in both space and musical
harmony; the fulfillment of this at the end was very satisfying
for most viewers.
Some General Conclusions.
music always invites an incitement to make narrative (in
music, there appears to be no real neutral -- we jump into narrative
faster than with visual art, for instance; perhaps the linear nature
of music compels us to create story );
the constant cross-referencing of both visual and oral stimuli
in a theatrical context deeply affects the generation of meaning
for the spectator, even with only trace elements of narrative available;
perceived stylistic and historical unity between text, music
and delivery tends to lead to a meaning based on shared cultural
assumptions;
when the narrative impulse and the musical impulse seem unified,
then we achieve a feeling and perception of consonance; WE WILL
CREATE CONSONANCE IF WE CAN;
relational imperatives of singing bodies in motion with each
other are not guided by the geometry of the music, but by character
and narrative impulses;
though we seek as viewer/listeners to generate continuous
theatrical meaning through the opening frame of sounds and
images presented to us, the organizing principle regarding meaning
and story may come later in our experience as sounds and images
pile up and resonate;
the impulse towards what is text or narrative is rooted in
what we see before us, not what we hear in the background; depending
on the stylistic unity, background sound either supports or destabilizes
the meaning generated onstage.
Some Future Considerations.
Based on this preliminary work, I would suggest three areas which
might prove fruitful for future researchers, or indeed, theatre
practitioners in a production situation:
Working with the notion of correlative and
disjunctive styles. How are scenes or narrative affected when paired
with music which shares stylistic traits, compared to being paired
with music which is noticeably different, challenging or unsettling?
(This is really working with musics noted associative qualities).
How does the repetition of a piece of music within a narrative
affect the reception of that narrative? This also works with the
idea of association, but one that is formally built into the particular
traffic of one staged work. If we hear a character sing a piece
of music in an initial context, how does that change the way we
generate meaning when the song is repeated in a later, very different
context?
The idea of darkness, or sightlessness, when combined with
singing, moving bodies on stage. How do we, as an audience, invest
in kinetic and relational meaning between characters when we can
no longer see them? How does singing affect the theatrical imagination
of the darkened audience what happens when aural clues become
the pre-eminent tools from which to craft meaning?
Lastly, it might be interesting to see some work that looks
even more specifically at live singing in contrast to both instrumental
and recorded music. According to some theorists, voice exists precisely
at the threshold between self and world, centre and periphery: this
seems an idea rich with theatrical possibility. A singing voice
radiating outward to others invites, insistently, that they attend
to and participate in it and the life it represents.
Martin Julien, May 2003
Sources
Music
Panis Angelicus Thomas Aquinas/Cesar Franck
Nanas Lied Brecht/Weill
(Antiphon No. 44) Hildegard von Bingen
Of the Fathers Love Begotten and Aurelius Prudentius
In EvangeliumMagnificat John Taverner
Sleepers Wake (Mein Freund ist Mein)
J.S. Bach
Tinas Song Don Horsburgh
Texts
Junky William S. Burroughs
The Journey of Ibn Fattouma Naguib Mahfouz
The Red Balloon Albert Lamorisse
Pilgermann Russell Hoban
Hockey Mom, Hockey Dad Michael Melski
The Toad and his Spots Latin/American Folktale
The Owl and the Pussycat E. Lear/S. Owens
Oprah Magazine "This Is What I Know"
- Sept. '03
Ages Al Purdy
Modes
Lydian, Dorian, Phrygian, Mixolydian, Locrian, Ionian
Bibliography
Music and Discourse: Toward a Semiology of Music,
Jean-Jaques Nattiez, Priceton University Press, 1990
The Interpretation of Music, Michael Krausz, Clarendon Press Oxford,
1993
Philosophical Perspectives on Music, Wayne D. Bowman, Oxford University
Press, 1998
Sketch of a New Esthetic of Music, Ferruccio Busoni, 1907
The Republic, Plato, 360 BCE
These research sessions were administered under the
provisions of the Canadian Theatre Agreement. Total number of hours
in sessions: Twenty-four.
Back to top ^
|
|